Tuesday, November 17, 2009
riana coan?
Monday, November 9, 2009
[dream] chicken or the egg?
Grab a pencil and paper, read the vignettes, and answer the questions below:
A young man goes into a store and desires a pack of chips. He looks carefully around him, puts the pack under his baggy shirt, and walks out of the store with the stolen goods.
A teen boy notices a man with a nice watch across the street. He puts his hooded sweatshirt on, walks across the street, and puts a weapon to the back of the man and demands his watch. The man gives it up, the boy places it on and walks away.
Write down the following descriptions for each scenario:
-His race.
-His age.
-The city in which he lives.
-The reason he committed the robbery.
According to the Moffit text, there may be different reasons underlying the motive of each boy. I initially wanted to delve into stereotypes that the reader may have had about who each of the males may have been, but I believe that discussion to be more effective in person. In the meantime, evaluate your own answers, and see if they were validated by the evidence within the article. It was of interest to me, morever, that Moffitt noted no differences between the race and class (among other factors) of offenders, and I would assume that many people do not believe, inherently, that this could be true, likely evidenced by the answers we would write to the above.
But, I'll move on. The title asks an important question: which part of the cycle contributes to such antisocial behavior? Is the neuropsychological nuances caused by the mother's malnutrition, poor neonatal care, and intoxicants ingested while pregnant? Considering most studies do not take place within the inner-city, it would be difficult to measure the differences experienced between the proportion of low-income children with that of middle- and high-class children. Considering the latter are the groups most often exposed to treatment, and therefore most-likely to be diagnosed with a pathology rather than a social stigma, it is difficult to see whether or not all groups are diagnosed with antisocial behavior in the same way, or even develop it in the same manner.
For example, would trauma be sufficient to affect the neurological component of a child born with relatively normal functioning? Studying Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) and Brain Tumor (BT) patients this summer, it is apparent that executive functioning abilities decrease after various treatments like chemotherapy or radiation. Studies indicate that a majority of TBI and BT patients never quite function at the same level of their peers, but we do not see them develop antisocial behavior disorders, even those with the negative environments. I therefore question if maladaptive parenting skills, which may be precipitated by pathology itself, may be a precursor to the neurological deficits that many of these antisocial children develop. And if this is the case, is it truly accurate to say that discrimination is not found between various social groups?
Or do these deficits increase the problems parents face in raising such children? The environmental factors that negatively impact these children is something that is found in many lax or overreactive parenting situations. Such factors contribute negatively to the behavioral outcomes of children, particularly males. When friends draw back, and parents, teachers, and other adults continue to penalize, a vicious cycle of negative socialization occurs. I am reminded of my students who were not "good" enough to go out on field trips. They were therefore chastised in class, not shown positive examples, and therefore more likely to commit such offenses again. This same socialization occurs within juvenile systems as well as jails, where we continue to offer criminals new negative social networks, as well as more opportunities to act upon their impulses with their new found knowledge. This environment, mixed with such neuropsychological deficits, leads to the taxonomical positions posed by Moffit in one of the most interesting articles we've read thus far!
I have soooo much to say about this article - I also can't wait to discuss how the differences between identity may shape the face of the pathology or diagnosis in class.
Sunday, November 1, 2009
fight or fright? (halloween edition...)
- Bees
- Balloons
- Needles (I would love for the alliteration to continue through the series, but it's more than just blood drawing - it's ALL needles)
Monday, October 26, 2009
when keeping it real goes wrong (remember that Dave Chapelle skit?)…
p.s. best benjabrunch/brunchamin EVER ladies (and ben, who is SURE to read this!! ;o) )
Saturday, October 17, 2009
eyes wide shut
1) The University of Michigan rocks.
2) Including super-huge portraits of each author in the article negates the fact that their writing is totally discombobulated and way too verbose.
Now, getting to the topic of stimulus control (SC) and stressors and abuses that lead to depression.
The techniques highlighted in the SC article as well as the sleep hygiene handout seemed to express the validity of using this therapy with sleep deprived clients. I only questioned how the average person would be able to stick to such a regime, or participate it in the first place. The therapeutic push, however, of suggesting that nothing else has worked so take a stab at this, was fairly convincing to me. I wish I knew of it when I used to suffer from insomnia in college. But then I worked for Teach For America. That would be the second most effective cure after SC, I'm sure.
I really enjoyed the Voelker article entitled Stress, Sleep Loss, and Substance Abuse Create Potent Recipe for College Depression not because it was published from Michigan (well, not solely because of that), rather it had a great mix of clinical and neuropsych, as well as social commentary. In general, our executive functioning allocates specific areas and hormones to address stressors that can rewire the brain to handle stress, but this action takes away from its original function. Meg commented in assessment yesterday that research appears to advocate less specific function related to one hemisphere only. This is true, but from what I can recall this summer in neuropsych research, when trauma occurs in one hemisphere, the other hemisphere can compensate for this lack by performing the duties once associated with its other half. This, understandably, burdens the lone hemisphere, so while there is certainly hemispherical interplay, there will be lessened functioning throughout the entire brain because one is inundated with double the tasks.
In reference to the depression model proposed by Voelker, this makes a world of sense. People who compensate coping with their psychiatric disorders by overindulging in substances are placing an abundant request on their brains to sort everything out, which likely only further complicates their disorder. This, coupled with social pressures and economic woes, leads to a lack of sleep, and in more recent cases, extreme violent behavior by those who are just unable to cope with the demands placed on their bodies and minds.
As stated, combing out the various kinks in our lives, i.e. monitoring the amount we eat, drink, sleep, and have time to address our maladaptive thoughts, will act as prevention for the large increase we're seeing in the obesity, substance abuse, insomnia, and depression in college campuses, which are sure to increase as a result of the flailing economy.
Monday, October 12, 2009
put me in [life] coach...i’m ready to play!
Sunday, October 4, 2009
you down with CBT? yeah you know me!
Our articles this week hinged around cognitive and behavioral therapies, both in the fashion of CBT (Cognitive Behavioral Therapy) and RE[B]T (Rational Emotive [Behavioral] Therapy). Of note, I found the Ellis article to be particularly amusing, in that the father of such a treatment seemed apologetic and provided a plethora of excuses or caveats throughout this entire article for why REBT is essentially CBT before CBT became what it is today!
In general, however, I was enthralled by the CBT articles, particularly that of the British Association for the Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies' Mapping Psychotherapy - What is CBT? This guideline presented the perfect framework for the type of treatment I would like to practice in inner-city communities where mindsets are often the largest factor keeping those citizens enslaved. Bob Marley speaks to us through song about this concept, urging residents to "emancipate yourselves from mental slavery/none but ourselves can free our minds." (Redemption Song)
The map shown explicitly in Appendix 1 on page 6 highlights the necessary methods both clients and therapists must engage in to yield results. Through our exploration of EBP/ESTs, we acknowledge that assessment allows opportunities to establish rapport, as supported by Rogerian theory, as well as time to postulate which CBT assessment would best fit our client. Formulation closely follows this assessment in order to discover the presenting problem of our client while giving us a baseline with which to measure our client's success. Intervention stems from our hypothesis and involves a trusting relationship between client and therapist, so that outside homework can be achieved by each individual in order to benefit the client seeking behavioral modification through cognitive shifts - i.e. attempting exposure or addressing a fallacious belief. Finally, evaluation measures the impact both participants in the CBT have had on the outcome of the client, and long-term effects can be measured during this stage as well.
These four stepping stones to behavior changes, as well as cognition restructuring, are key to urban youth developing a more positive attitude about the impact that they can make in this world. As Butler et al's work displays, CBT is highly effective in treating adolescent depression and anxiety disorders. If children who live in these areas are constantly exposed to negative rhetoric about their existence, it is natural to assume that they would internalize these messages. CBT works for this age population, so it is worth the effort to administer this form of treatment on such groups. It will be interesting to note whether this treatment works across racial and class lines in such strong effect sizes.
The lab I work in for my research conducts a longitudinal study on the effectiveness of a prevention program targeting the parents of children with poor behavioral outcomes. Reading these articles really leads me to believe that as the children increase in age, offering CBT directly to some of the children will allow us to compare whether this type of intervention, mixed with preventative measures, will truly minimize the risk associated with growing up in inner-city neighborhoods. If this is the case, we may be able to revolutionize the way children within these environments perceive the world around them, thereby affecting both behavior and cognition - the basis of CBT!
Thursday, October 1, 2009
...
but i really can't even speak after watching the video within this blog:
http://ow.ly/rDn6
speechless.
-R
Tuesday, September 22, 2009
it is the relationship, stupid!
1) If things are so obvious (i.e. ridding ourselves of theory and forging stronger alliances with a client), why are they not being done?
To whit I am happy to answer! I will juxtapose my psychology life with that of my political and teaching (former!) lives respectively.
As the title of this blog indicates, Kirschenbaum & Jourdan take a stab at the Clinton-era rhetoric of “It is the economy, stupid!” This fact seemed very apparent to Clinton advisors advocating for change, but for those who appreciate the status quo, i.e. those in power or with authority, why would change be necessary? In many ways, that’s admitting to the failures you’ve already done in office as well as those you’re promulgating currently, so in essence, you’re giving several reasons why you’re incompetent. Face it, politics, after all, IS all about getting re-elected…
Well, so to is the real world, in which psychologists all over are having extremely difficult times admitting to their clinical errors in working with their patients in a very paternalistic way, placing demands, theories, and methods of therapy on a client that do not work as well as others. That would be an admission that for all of these years, the training, hard work, and most importantly, knowledge the clinician obtained and outputted would be less effective than another concept. This, by itself, is a large reason why many clinicians not trained on a model of alliance would rather default to their previous training than risk being the martyr or proponent of a new cause (which, as explored in previous topics, may not have been empirically proven while going through its beginning stages in the field – after all, it takes quite a few years to gain momentum, significance, and retraining).
And, in a dorky selfish sidenote for point number two:
2) How I truly appreciate the way in which this class is organized, for the information we are reading about continues to build upon each other!
As a (former) teacher, it is very important to me that knowledge be built linearly – if we are taught something, it should be in a logical sequence that would strengthen the former concepts. That is precisely what this coursework is offering! From the beginning of our class, we have continued to look at articles that help to explain both the former and latter weeks. These articles truly built upon the concepts of paternalism vs. liberalism, clinically-based vs. empirically-based, effect-size vs. significance, etc. When reweaving these themes throughout the fabric of this class, it not only strengthens our understanding, but makes us more competent to discuss these issues in ways that would not be attained had the articles had no relevance to each other.
This was clear for me when I read K & J’s citation of Elliott’s 2003 paper as he wrote, “Working effectively with clients requires adapting the therapist’s approach to the client’s general presenting problems, the within-session task, and the client’s immediate experience in the moment.” This immediately triggered sentiments of Consumer Reports, liberalism, and efficacy. I am not only getting stronger in my arguments, but I tend to understand many sides now, and can debate from either stance.
To bring it all together, the relationship between the client and the therapist is important. This we know. But what we may not know is why therapists may be hesitant to adopt such a model (I posit that it’s due to job security, as are all things in life). We are additionally fortunate to have a method of learning that advocates many sides in a systemic building block format, allowing us to gain knowledge of multiple subjects in objective manners over the course of the semester. I appreciate this method and look forward to addressing alliance more in class, since statistically speaking, I’ll likely be called (right probability proponents?).
Sunday, September 13, 2009
I've got a theory...get over it!
First and foremost, shout out to the JAM JcRew for the most awesome birthday surprise EVER! :o) Totally caught me off guard, and as you can see from the pic, I was extremely happy! Go team!
Moving forth to the extremely dense readings this week...
Looking at Smith's article, "The end of theoretical orientations?", really makes me wonder whether or not I have entered into an archaic profession resistant to change. I think most people can reasonably agree that therapy, whether it be physical, psychological, or medical, truly benefits the patient if done correctly. In our field, however, we spend so much time worrying about who created the concept of our theory, or which trendy wave we entered into graduate school on, but are they necessary to complete the treatment of our patient, which is arguably the top priority.
In looking at other fields, do we really require them to highlight a certain technique with their treatment? Certainly, there are specialties within other fields - we need to know which portion of our body needs healing, so we go to an expert who can assist us with that. However, when we want our mind healed, there is a sudden shift to which way the professional is going to treat us. Now here's the conundrum: no two people have the same exact pathologies (as studied earlier with diagnostic categorization arguments), so should WE use one school of thought on THEM? Shouldn't the client dictate what type of practice we run?
The shift towards eclecticism accounts for this new way of thinking. I'll give the old geezers the benefit of the doubt - afterall, we are one of the newer sciences. But after a while, don't you think someone should have recognized that the round ball doesn't always fit inside of the square peg? One size does not fit all, so we should not think that one therapy would fit our increasingly more complex client need. (Tangentially, the concept of studying in graduate school under a certain school of thought [which would dictate where you wanted to study], coupled with the search for the perfect advisor relationship is just too much for an applicant to handle - we need to do away with such strain in the Ph.D. quest! Geesh!)
In essence, realizing that our main goal is to "create more smiles in the world" will guide us towards being better clinicians. Learning how to treat various symptoms as they are presented as well as being instructed in various methods of psychological theory will lend to more satisfied consumers and healthier minds.
Sunday, September 6, 2009
One Flew Over the Cuckoo's Nest...
The Dodo Bird theory was easily struck down throughout several meta-analytical studies presented in the work. What is most interesting to me is the initial theory of all psychological treatments having equivalent effects. I can only think to compare that to the educational system, in noting that regardless of the teacher, curriculum, and child within the classroom, all results are going to appear to be the same. I can assure you that that is hardly the case, as a former teacher, because so many variables affect the outcome of quality teaching and instruction. If this can be seen in areas of expertise ranging from the classroom to the emergency room, why would my clinic be any different? How could one even postulate that these various treatments would yield the same result?
If in fact equivalence was the end result of all clinician and treatment procedures, I find it highly unlikely that we (future clinicians) would be fighting so diligently for a spot to practice this art. Additionally, I believe that less trained professionals would be able to administer treatments to patients, considering our training would be irrelevant to their outcome. There has been a push in recent years over this (especially Masters level therapy in some states to reduce costs of mental health care), but a large pushback from professionals who can show through empirical evidence that the quality of the training of the clinician, paired with empirically relevant training models, yields better patient outcomes than the alternative.
I would agree hardily, therefore, that we must strive for results that benefit the patients and are replicable between and within treatments in order to solidify the effectiveness of highly-trained therapists and various treatments. Working towards this goal will keep our profession intact, and any potential clinicians from flying the coop…
Tuesday, September 1, 2009
Epic Fail!
Greetings all!
I'm UBER informal, and blogging surely doesn't help, so I will do my best to get back on the professional track starting...oh wait! here's the JAM _R (we love you Jess!) at din din Sunday! k...now!
I enjoyed both articles, but Persons resonated more with me. I've never heard of studying psychological phenomena rather than psychiatric diagnoses, but it certainly makes sense given one point that he states on 1253 (page 2). Persons very clearly outlines the case for schizophrenics who may not have thought disorder, and those without schizophrenia having thought disorder. This immediately triggered my memory for false negatives/positives. I thought, almost instinctively, that it must be worse to have the false negative, because you're walking around with a disease/pathology and not know it. But as I voiced my concerns aloud, my friend very wonderfully suggested that a false positive would be worse psychologically. Come to think of it, he's absolutely right, and summed up both articles very well.
Both Persons and Widiger & Clark suggest that a person is NOT going to miss out on the fact that s/he has symptoms indicative of pathology. For example, thought disorder or a low IQ can be symptomatic of schizophrenia and mental retardation, respectively, but do not necessarily imply either disorder. The inverse, however, is to give someone the label of schizophrenic or mentally retarded, which are stigmatized in any community, when in reality, s/he is void of such markers as thought disorder or may have genetic abnormalities such as Down's Syndrome. Although the person absent of diagnosis is still aware of his/her symptoms, the person with the misdiagnosis may be traumatized by such a label, and further not helped with his/her presenting problem, since a referral to another psychologist may result in treatment for the typical diagnostic problem of those patients.
We must remember, as clinicians, that our patients need care for his/her concerns, not for a cookie cutter solution to get him/her out of our office. Large bins that our clients go in may be a better tool for the average over-burdened employee, but what about the individual? Sure labels are easy to apply, but are they easy to get off?
-R.
As a reference, from our good pal Wikipedia...
*Type I (α) - false positive: reject the null hypothesis when the null hypothesis is true, and
*Type II (β) - false negative: fail to reject the null hypothesis when the null hypothesis is false